The New IQ: Use Your Working Memory to Think Stronger, Smarter, Faster. Tracy AllowayЧитать онлайн книгу.
time and made more mistakes. The high-fat diet apparently diminished their working memory ability.
Other theories about specific foods that turn the brain reward system against us abound. In 2009, David Kessler suggested that it’s foods that combine salt, sugar, and fat that do a number on the brain’s reward system and lead to overconsumption and obesity. Sugar has also been implicated on its own, including high-fructose corn syrup, maltose, dextrose, and dozens of other sugar sources found in our food supply. As scientists attempt to pinpoint a single food, category of food, or combination of foods that trip up our reward system, it seems that it’s when we overindulge in these foods that it becomes a problem. After all, it is important to remember that the human body needs fat for a healthy brain, needs glucose to think clearly, and needs salt. But when we overindulge, our working memory suffers.
When the Feel-Good Neurotransmitter Doesn’t Feel Good
At the other end of the eating disorder spectrum are people with anorexia nervosa. Anna Patterson, a young woman who blogs about anorexia, was convinced that despite weighing eighty-nine pounds, she still needed to get rid of her “fat” stomach, and although she refused to eat, she couldn’t stop fixating on food.
While it appears that people who eat too much do so at least in part because they’ve become addicted to the dopamine hit they get from an order of french fries or a piece of crispy bacon, new research has shown that people with anorexia don’t respond the same way to dopamine. In 2012, Walter Kaye compared the brain activity of recovering anorexic women with a healthy group of women of the same age and used a PET scanner to determine the effect of dopamine in their brains. He gave both groups of women a single oral dose of the drug amphetamine, which resulted in a big dopamine release.
For most people, the dopamine hit equals pleasure, and as expected, the healthy women experienced feelings of pleasure and euphoria. When Kaye looked at the scans of the healthy women, he saw activation in an area containing the nucleus accumbens, which is filled with dopamine receptors.
Conversely, the brain scans of the recovering anorexics revealed activation in the dorsal caudate, a part of the brain that worries about consequences. In other words, for anorexic individuals, pleasure automatically brings feelings of guilt and worry. Sure enough, when the anorexic women were asked to complete a questionnaire on anxiety, they showed very high levels, lasting over three hours after having received the dopamine hit. For them, their out-of-control behavior was not about pursuing pleasure but rather a desire to avoid the feelings of guilt and anxiety that accompany the dopamine release that comes with eating.
Being “addicted to starvation,” as biologist Valerie Campan calls it, or simply being hooked on self-control is also linked to working memory problems. It seems that too little pleasure can become just as addictive as too much, and working memory suffers in both extremes. In 2006, Australian psychologist Eva Kemps recruited a group of anorexic women and healthy women, and gave them a series of tests as well as a questionnaire about food. She found that the anorexic women reported having more intrusive thoughts about food, weight, and body shape. And while the anorexics and healthy women had similar IQ scores, she argued that the anorexics suffered from a diminished working memory.
In 2009, Arne Zastrow and colleagues at the University of Heidelberg took this further to look at brain scans of women with anorexia. They recruited a group of fifteen anorexic women and fifteen healthy women and asked them to perform a task that involved processing and monitoring information while they were in a brain scanner. The task was to keep a target shape in mind, like a circle or triangle, and press a button to identify this shape from other shapes. Another component of the task was that the target shape would change so the women had to mentally discard the previous shape and update their working memory with the new target. Clinicians commonly use this cognitive task to measure how quickly people can adapt when the rules of the game change.
As in the Kemp study, the anorexic women made more errors than their healthy counterparts, indicating cognitive inflexibility: they find it difficult to disengage from one idea and move to another, which may explain why they display rigidity in behavior. As you recall, a strong working memory helps you shift your focus, suggesting that working memory is impaired in people with anorexia. It is likely part of the reason that it is so hard for people with this disorder to shift from the notion that food is “bad” and to start thinking of it as something enjoyable that promotes good health.
The brain scans in Zastrow’s study offered some insight into why this happens. The scans showed that a number of brain areas involved with motivation were underactive in the anorexic women. The healthy women also showed greater activation in their PFC, demonstrating that they were eliciting their working memory to solve the task. But the anorexic women did not show any activation in the PFC, suggesting that their working memory was on cruise control.
The disadvantages of a poor working memory are clear: when our Conductor falters we realize just how vital it is for our financial, psychological, and physical well-being. Having a poor working memory is linked to a greater risk for unhealthy habits and behaviors that can leave you bankrupt, addicted, obese, or all of these. And to exacerbate the problem, out-of-control behaviors may also drain your working memory or even recruit it to work against your best interests.
The Most Important Learning Tool
IQ SCORES HAVE LONG been held as the benchmark for academic success. But in our own research, we found that relying on IQ to predict success is flawed.
When Tracy began investigating working memory, IQ, and academic achievement, she wanted to find out which cognitive skills were most important in predicting a child’s success in school. For one of her first published studies, she recruited almost two hundred kindergartners and gave them a variety of tests, including working memory and IQ tests. When she compared their scores on these tests with their grades, she was taken aback by the results.
The students’ IQ scores were surprisingly inaccurate in determining how well they would do in school, contrary to what you might think. According to her findings, students can have an average IQ score but perform poorly in school. For example, one of the kindergartners in this study, Andrew, had an average IQ score, but by the time he reached second grade, he was struggling to keep up with his classmates. If IQ was a good predictor of success, then Andrew shouldn’t have had such a tough time in school. His IQ wasn’t the reason he was doing poorly.
When Tracy looked at Andrew’s working memory score, she noticed that it was low compared to his peers. In fact, if you stood him in a line of one hundred children of the same age, Andrew would be at the end of that line when it came to his working memory skills. Andrew’s poor working memory score correlated to his poor performance in school.
The same held true for the other kindergartners. Tracy found that working memory could predict what grade students would get with far greater accuracy than IQ. In fact, if Tracy knew a child’s working memory, she could determine his or her academic skills with 95 percent accuracy. When we looked at the youngsters’ grades six years later, we found that working memory had such a powerful impact on learning that by knowing their working memory in kindergarten, we could also predict with 95 percent accuracy what their grades would be in the sixth grade.
In another study, Tracy wanted to identify the key cognitive skill required for success in learning the most basic school subjects: reading, comprehension, spelling, and math. She tracked nearly seventy students aged seven to eleven for two years. She tested their working memory and IQ and then compared those scores with their academic achievement in these four subjects. When she analyzed the data and compared working memory to IQ, she once again found that IQ contributed very little to achievement. On the other hand, working memory was the most important cognitive