Эротические рассказы

Essentials of Veterinary Ophthalmology. Kirk N. GelattЧитать онлайн книгу.

Essentials of Veterinary Ophthalmology - Kirk N. Gelatt


Скачать книгу
to an 80‐fold increase in humans.

      Dark Adaptation

      Photopic Vision

      Light Adaptation

      Pupil

      As with scotopic vision, the pupil also contributes to photopic vision because miosis protects the retina from excessive and harmful amounts of light. It is proposed that the large corpora nigra found on the superior (and often the inferior pupil also) border of the iris in some species provides additional protection as it decreases the amount of light entering the eye from the superior visual field (where the Sun is located), further reducing glare and improving vision in bright light. Moreover, because a miotic slit pupil can block light more efficiently than a miotic circular one, it is suggested that slit pupils have evolved in nocturnal or crepuscular species such as cats and geckos that need to function in daytime.

Schematic illustration of a comprehensive canine ERG protocol, following preparation of the animal in ambient light, the light is turned off.

      Temporal responsiveness of the retina has two aspects: motion detection and flicker detection. The retina responds to flashes of light as long as there is sufficient interval between two consecutive flashes, allowing the retina to recover from one response before the next flash is presented. However, as the frequency of these flashes increases, a point is reached at which the retina does not have enough time to recover between flashes and therefore it can no longer distinguish the individual flashes. At this point, which is termed the critical flicker frequency (CFF) or flicker fusion frequency, the eye perceives a steady light even though this light is made up of numerous flickers. There are separate CFFs for rod‐driven responses and signals generated from the different cone types, just as there are separate adaptation mechanisms for the rod and cone systems.

      Moving through the environment produces a flow of images projected onto the photoreceptors, resulting in huge amounts of visual information necessary for navigating through a complex environment. Perception of motion is required for both directing visual attention to a certain location in the visual field and segmenting moving objects from their background. Psychophysically, there are various ways of discerning motion: (i) movement can be perceived if an object is moving across our visual field while our eyes and head are stationary; and (ii) motion can also be perceived when either the head or the eyes are moved to pursue a moving object.

      It is rather extraordinary to note that all vertebrate species, and many invertebrates, have two eyes. Having more than one eye is required for a large visual field, virtually 360° in some species, and for stereopsis, or depth perception. However, no more than two eyes are required to achieve these aims.

      Visual Fields

      On the other hand, most primate and predator species have frontal eyes. In these species, most of the frontal visual field is covered by a binocular vision; there are two small, peripheral monocular fields and a large blind spot behind the skull. Therefore, enucleation will cause a cat, for example, to lose approximately 30° of one visual field, while in a horse the same procedure will cause a loss of about 145°.

      Large (monocular) visual fields are typically associated with prey species that need to detect predators. This is why these species usually have a pupil and a visual streak whose shapes are aligned with the horizon, allowing them to identify predators and conspecifics.

      Stereopsis

Image described by caption.

      Geometry and Retinal Disparity


Скачать книгу
Яндекс.Метрика