Эротические рассказы

Attachment Theory and Research. Группа авторовЧитать онлайн книгу.

Attachment Theory and Research - Группа авторов


Скачать книгу
href="#ulink_4e0196cb-8e7c-581a-8d7b-fd0a732f14f2">20 It is my belief that it is situations such as these, rather than the frustration of oral desires, that engender the most frequent and intense hostility in infants and young children, hostility, moreover, which is inevitably directed towards the loved object itself. This is of the greatest relevance when we come to consider why in some children expectant anxiety in regard to separation exists at a level above the normal.

      The second is that the period when they are most active is also the period when patterns of control and of regulating conflict are being laid down. Our data demonstrate that when primary anxiety arising from separation is allowed to persist, defences of a primitive nature (such as those giving rise to detachment described earlier) come into play. There is reason to suppose that the early and intense activation of such defensive processes may create patterns which in later life are of pathogenic significance. This is a theme I have touched on in an earlier paper in connexion with critical phases of development (Bowlby, 1957) and which I hope to pursue further.

      Whether or not these reasons prove to be the right ones, there can be little doubt that separation anxiety is an exceedingly common component of neurotic anxiety. This was early recognized by Freud. ‘One of the clearest indications that a child will later become neurotic’, he observed, ‘is to be seen in an insatiable demand for his parents’ affection’ (Freud, 1905, p. 223); this, of course, is another way of describing the child who exhibits, in excess, expectant anxiety in regard to separation and loss of love. Few would dispute this view today. There are, however, several hypotheses current in regard to why some children develop in this way and others do not; and it is in fact on this issue that the views advanced here differ most from those of Freud.

      1 Constitutional FactorsSome ‘children have inherently a greater amount of libidinal need in their constitution than others,’ and so are more sensitive than others to an absence of gratification (Freud, 1917).Some children have inherently a stronger death instinct than others, which manifests itself in unusually strong persecutory and depressive anxiety (Klein, 1932).

      2 Environmental FactorsVariations in the birth process and severe traumata occurring during the first weeks of post‐natal life may increase the (organic) anxiety response and heighten the anxiety potential, thereby causing a more severe reaction to later (psychological) dangers met with in life (Greenacre, [1941] 1952, [1945] 1952).Some children are ‘spoiled’ by excess of early libidinal gratification: they therefore demand more of it and, when not gratified, miss it more (Freud, 1905, 1917, 1926).Some children are made excessively sensitive to the possibility of separation or loss of love either through the experience of actual separation (Edleston, 1943; Bowlby, 1951), or through the use of separation or loss of love as a threat (Suttie, 1935; Fairbairn, [1941] 1952).

      It should be noted that whereas hypotheses 1 (a), 2 (b) and 2 (c) are framed to account for the liability to an excess in particular of separation anxiety, 1 (b) and 2 (a) are intended to account for the liability to an excess of anxiety of any kind.

      I do not believe there is any clear evidence in support of the first four of these hypotheses. Since with our present research techniques there is no way of determining differences in constitutional endowment, the first pair unavoidably remain untested (though of course not disproved). As regards the next pair, the evidence in regard to 2 (a) is far from clear; indeed in her paper Phyllis Greenacre is careful to explain that she regards it as no more than a plausible hypothesis. Evidence in regard to 2 (b) seems at the best equivocal: the subjection of a child to neurotic overprotection or to excessive libidinal demands from his mother sometimes appears like excess of affection but clearly cannot be equated with it. Evidence in regard to the fifth hypothesis, 2 (c), however, is abundant and affirmative. Therefore, without necessarily rejecting the first four, the fifth hypothesis, that an excess of separation anxiety may be due either to an experience of actual separation or to threats of separation, rejection, or loss of love, can be adopted with confidence. Probably a majority of analysts today utilize it in their work in some degree.

      It is strange that in his writings Freud practically never invoked it. On the contrary, in addition to postulating hypothesis 1 (a), that some children have a constitutionally greater need of libidinal gratification than others, he committed himself early and consistently to hypothesis 2 (b), that an excess of separation anxiety is due to an excess of parental affection – in other words, the traditional theory of spoiling. Thus in the Three Essays (1905), after commending the mother who strokes, rocks, and kisses her child and thereby teaches him to love, he nevertheless warns against excess: ‘An excess of parental affection does harm by causing precocious sexual maturity and also because, by spoiling the child, it makes him incapable in later life of temporarily doing without love or of being content with a smaller amount of it’ (p. 223). The same theme runs through much of his theorizing about Little Hans (1909), though it is in his discussion of this small boy’s separation anxiety that he comes nearest the view adopted here: he attributes part of it to the fact that Little Hans had been separated from his mother at the time of his baby sister’s birth (pp. 114 and 132). However, both in the Introductory Lectures (1917, p. 340) and in Inhibitions, Symptoms and Anxiety (1926, p. 167) he makes no reference to such origins and instead explicitly adopts the theory of spoiling.

      In addition to this, it seems probable that another reason for Freud’s misperception of the origins of excessive separation anxiety was the delay in his recognition of the close bond of child to mother and the length of time over which it normally persists at high intensity; only if the child’s strong attachment is perceived as normal is its severance or threat of severance recognized as dangerous. It is true that by the time he wrote Inhibitions, Symptoms and Anxiety he was of opinion that a main cause of man’s proneness to neurosis lies in ‘the long period of time during which the young of the human species is in a condition of helplessness and dependence … (which) establishes the earliest situations of danger and creates the need to be loved’ (1926, pp. 154–155). Yet, so far as I know, he never drew from this the natural conclusion that disruptions or threats of disruption of the primary bond are likely to prove a major hazard.

      It will thus be seen that the views advanced in this paper differ from Freud’s not so much on the nature of separation anxiety itself but on the conditions which determine its presence in excessive degree. On this issue indeed the two views are the opposite of one another. It is perhaps because of this and because Freud’s hypothesis of spoiling has been built deep into psycho‐analytic theory that there has been so much reluctance in many analysts to accept as valid the evidence which supports the hypothesis here advanced. It is time to return to this.

      In my view the best opportunity for uncovering the conditions which lead an individual to


Скачать книгу
Яндекс.Метрика